References

Atkinson, B. M., Smulders, T. V., & Wallenberg, J. C. (2017). An endocrine basis for tomboy identity: The second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) in tomboys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 79, 9–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.032
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475
Cinelli, C., Forney, A., & Pearl, J. (2024). A Crash Course in Good and Bad Controls. Sociological Methods and Research, 53(3), 1071–1104. https://doi.org/10.1177/00491241221099552
Daston, L. (2022). Rules: A short history of what we live by. Princeton University Press.
Daubenmier, J., Lin, J., Blackburn, E., Hecht, F. M., Kristeller, J., Maninger, N., Kuwata, M., Bacchetti, P., Havel, P. J., & Epel, E. (2011). Changes in stress, eating, and metabolic factors are related to changes in telomerase activity in a randomized mindfulness intervention pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(7), 917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.10.008
Davis, A. J., Cohen, E., & Nettle, D. (2025). Associations amongst poverty, loneliness, and a defensive symptom cluster characterised by pain, fatigue, and low mood. Public Health, 242, 272–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2025.02.037
Finkelstein, A., Taubman, S., Wright, B., Bernstein, M., Gruber, J., Newhouse, J. P., Allen, H., Baicker, K., & Group, O. H. S. (2012). The oregon health insurance experiment: Evidence from the first year. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 127(3), 1057–1106. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjs020
Fitouchi, L., & Nettle, D. (2025). Harmless bodily pleasures are moralized because they are perceived as reducing self-control and cooperativeness. Cognition, 262, 106154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2025.106154
Giner-Sorolla, R., Montoya, A. K., Reifman, A., Carpenter, T., Lewis, N. A., Aberson, C. L., Bostyn, D. H., Conrique, B. G., Ng, B. W., Schoemann, A. M., & Soderberg, C. (2024). Power to Detect What? Considerations for Planning and Evaluating Sample Size. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10888683241228328. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683241228328
Goh, J. X., Hall, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2016). Mini Meta-Analysis of Your Own Studies: Some Arguments on Why and a Primer on How. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10(10), 535–549. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12267
Grosz, M. P., Rohrer, J. M., & Thoemmes, F. (2020). The Taboo Against Explicit Causal Inference in Nonexperimental Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(5), 1243–1255. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620921521
Ho, R. T. H., Chan, J. S. M., Wang, C.-W., Lau, B. W. M., So, K. F., Yuen, L. P., Sham, J. S. T., & Chan, C. L. W. (2012). A randomized controlled trial of qigong exercise on fatigue symptoms, functioning, and telomerase activity in persons with chronic fatigue or chronic fatigue syndrome. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 44(2), 160–170. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-012-9381-6
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why Most Published Research Findings Are False. PLOS Medicine, 2(8), e124. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
Lakens, D. (2024). When and how to deviate from a preregistration. Collabra: Psychology, 10(1), 117094. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.117094
Lakens, D., Scheel, A. M., & Isager, P. M. (2018). Equivalence testing for psychological research: A tutorial. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(2), 259–269. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918770963
Langsrud, Ø. (2003). ANOVA for unbalanced data: Use Type II instead of Type III sums of squares. Statistics and Computing, 13(2), 163–167. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023260610025
Lavretsky, H., Epel, E. s., Siddarth, P., Nazarian, N., Cyr, N. St., Khalsa, D. s., Lin, J., Blackburn, E., & Irwin, M. r. (2013). A pilot study of yogic meditation for family dementia caregivers with depressive symptoms: effects on mental health, cognition, and telomerase activity. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(1), 57–65. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.3790
Lundberg, I., Johnson, R., & Stewart, B. M. (2021). What Is Your Estimand? Defining the Target Quantity Connects Statistical Evidence to Theory. American Sociological Review, 86(3), 532–565. https://doi.org/10.1177/00031224211004187
Møller, A. P. (1988). Female choice selects for male sexual tail ornaments in the monogamous swallow. Nature, 332(6165), 640–642. https://doi.org/10.1038/332640a0
Montgomery, J. M., Nyhan, B., & Torres, M. (2018). How Conditioning on Posttreatment Variables Can Ruin Your Experiment and What to Do about It. American Journal of Political Science, 62(3), 760–775. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12357
Nettle, D., Andrews, C., Reichert, S., Bedford, T., Kolenda, C., Parker, C., Martin-Ruiz, C., Monaghan, P., & Bateson, M. (2017). Early-life adversity accelerates cellular ageing and affects adult inflammation: Experimental evidence from the European starling. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 40794. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep40794
Nettle, D., Chevallier, C., Courson, B. de, Johnson, E. A., Johnson, M. T., & Pickett, K. E. (2025). Short-term changes in financial situation have immediate mental health consequences: Implications for social policy. Social Policy & Administration. https://doi.org/10.1111/spol.13065
Nettle, D., & Saxe, R. (2020). Preferences for redistribution are sensitive to perceived luck, social homogeneity, war and scarcity. Cognition, 198, 104234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104234
Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716
Paál, T., Carpenter, T., & Nettle, D. (2015). Childhood socioeconomic deprivation, but not current mood, is associated with behavioural disinhibition in adults. PeerJ, 3, e964. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.964
Rohrer, J. M. (2018). Thinking Clearly About Correlations and Causation: Graphical Causal Models for Observational Data. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(1), 27–42. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245917745629
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359–1366. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417632
Steegen, S., Tuerlinckx, F., Gelman, A., & Vanpaemel, W. (2016). Increasing Transparency Through a Multiverse Analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(5), 702–712. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616658637
Wickham, H. (2014). Tidy Data. Journal of Statistical Software, 59, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v059.i10
Wysocki, A. C., Lawson, K. M., & Rhemtulla, M. (2022). Statistical Control Requires Causal Justification. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 5(2), 25152459221095823. https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221095823